Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 15 de 15
Filtrar
1.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 8(1): 10, 2023 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36723822

RESUMEN

With a brief half-second presentation, a medical expert can determine at above chance levels whether a medical scan she sees is abnormal based on a first impression arising from an initial global image process, termed "gist." The nature of gist processing is debated but this debate stems from results in medical experts who have years of perceptual experience. The aim of the present study was to determine if gist processing for medical images occurs in naïve (non-medically trained) participants who received a brief perceptual training and to tease apart the nature of that gist signal. We trained 20 naïve participants on a brief perceptual-adaptive training of histology images. After training, naïve observers were able to obtain abnormality detection and abnormality categorization above chance, from a brief 500 ms masked presentation of a histology image, hence showing "gist." The global signal demonstrated in perceptually trained naïve participants demonstrated multiple dissociable components, with some of these components relating to how rapidly naïve participants learned a normal template during perceptual learning. We suggest that multiple gist signals are present when experts view medical images derived from the tens of thousands of images that they are exposed to throughout their training and careers. We also suggest that a directed learning of a normal template may produce better abnormality detection and identification in radiologists and pathologists.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Radiólogos , Femenino , Humanos
2.
Rev. colomb. psiquiatr ; 48(2): 96-104, ene.-jun. 2019. tab, graf
Artículo en Español | LILACS, COLNAL | ID: biblio-1042853

RESUMEN

RESUMEN Objetivo: Colombia está enfrentado una epidemia emergente del consumo endovenoso de heroína. Un conocimiento de los programas existentes que ofrecen tratamiento asistido con metadona en el país es necesario para que se puedan proponer estrategias de mejoría. Métodos: Se encuestaron 13 programas de regiones prioritarias. Se evaluaron las caracte rísticas demográficas y clínicas de los usuarios, así como los servicios ofrecidos por estos programas, sus protocolos de tratamiento con metadona y las diferencias en las barreras al tratamiento y las causas de abandono del tratamiento. Resultados: Se analizaron 12/13 cuestionarios, con un total de 538 pacientes activos. La mayo ría de los pacientes eran varones (85,5%) de 18 a 34 arios (70%). El 40% eran usuarios de drogas intravenosas y el 25% admitió compartir agujas. Entre las comorbilidades asociadas con el consumo de heroína se encontró la enfermedad mental (48%), la hepatitis C (8,7%) y la infección por el VIH (2%). La comorbilidad psiquiátrica se asocia más con los pacientes que acuden al sector privado (el 69,8 frente al 29,7%; p < 0,03). La media de la dosis inicial de metadona es 25,3 ± 8,9mg/día y las dosis de mantenimiento van de 41 a 80 mg/día. La falta de articulación con atención primaria fue una barrera más sentida que los problemas con la cobertura del seguro médico y los prejuicios del tratamiento con metadona (p < 0,05). También, los problemas administrativos y de la aseguradora (p < 0,003), la falta de sumi nistro de metadona (p < 0,018) y la recaída en el consumo (p < 0,014) son las razones más significativas de abandono del tratamiento. Conclusiones: Estos programas tienen diferentes niveles de desarrollo e implementación en los protocolos de tratamiento. Algunas de las barreras de acceso y de las causas de abandono del tratamiento pueden mitigarse mejorando la administración de salud.


ABSTRACT Objective: Colombia is facing a rising epidemic of intravenous heroin use. Knowledge of the methadone-assisted treatment programs in the country is crucial in order to propose improvement strategies. Methods: 13 programmes from priority regions were surveyed. The demographic and clinical characteristics of the patients attending the programs, a description of the services offered, their methadone treatment protocols, the various barriers to treatment and the causes of treatment abandonment were reviewed. Results: 12/13 questionnaires were analysed with a total of 538 active patients. Most of the patients attending these programs were men (85.5%) between 18 and 34 years-old (70%). Forty percent (40%) were intravenous drug users and 25% admitted sharing needles. The comorbidities associated with heroin use were mental illness (48%), hepatitis C (8.7%) and HIV (2%). Psychiatric comorbidity was more likely in patients attending the private sec tor (69.8% vs 29.7%; p < 0.03). The initial average dose of methadone administered was 25.3 ± 8.9mg/day, with a maintenance dose ranging from 41 to 80mg/day. Lack of align ment with primary care was perceived to be the most serious barrier to access, ahead of problems with insurance and prejudice towards treatment with methadone (p < 0.05). Health Administration and insurance problems (p < 0.003), together with the lack of availa bility of methadone (p < 0.018) and relapse (p < 0.014) were the most important reasons for abandonment of treatment. Conclusions: The treatment protocols of these programmes offer different levels of develop ment and implementation. Some of the barriers to access and reasons for abandonment of treatment with methadone can be mitigated with better health administration.


Asunto(s)
Humanos , Masculino , Adolescente , Adulto , Consumidores de Drogas , Dependencia de Heroína , Metadona , Atención Primaria de Salud , Recurrencia , Terapéutica , Comorbilidad , Protocolos Clínicos , Colombia , Heroína , Administración en Salud
3.
Rev Colomb Psiquiatr (Engl Ed) ; 48(2): 96-104, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés, Español | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30981333

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Colombia is facing a rising epidemic of intravenous heroin use. Knowledge of the methadone-assisted treatment programs in the country is crucial in order to propose improvement strategies. METHODS: 13 programmes from priority regions were surveyed. The demographic and clinical characteristics of the patients attending the programs, a description of the services offered, their methadone treatment protocols, the various barriers to treatment and the causes of treatment abandonment were reviewed. RESULTS: 12/13 questionnaires were analysed with a total of 538 active patients. Most of the patients attending these programs were men (85.5%) between 18 and 34 years-old (70%). Forty percent (40%) were intravenous drug users and 25% admitted sharing needles. The comorbidities associated with heroin use were mental illness (48%), hepatitis C (8.7%) and HIV (2%). Psychiatric comorbidity was more likely in patients attending the private sector (69.8% vs 29.7%; p<0.03). The initial average dose of methadone administered was 25.3±8.9mg/day, with a maintenance dose ranging from 41 to 80mg/day. Lack of alignment with primary care was perceived to be the most serious barrier to access, ahead of problems with insurance and prejudice towards treatment with methadone (p<0.05). Health Administration and insurance problems (p<0.003), together with the lack of availability of methadone (p<0.018) and relapse (p<0.014) were the most important reasons for abandonment of treatment. CONCLUSIONS: The treatment protocols of these programmes offer different levels of development and implementation. Some of the barriers to access and reasons for abandonment of treatment with methadone can be mitigated with better health administration.


Asunto(s)
Accesibilidad a los Servicios de Salud , Dependencia de Heroína/epidemiología , Metadona/administración & dosificación , Abuso de Sustancias por Vía Intravenosa/epidemiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Colombia , Femenino , Dependencia de Heroína/rehabilitación , Humanos , Masculino , Compartición de Agujas/estadística & datos numéricos , Tratamiento de Sustitución de Opiáceos/métodos , Abuso de Sustancias por Vía Intravenosa/rehabilitación , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
4.
Psychiatr Serv ; 70(2): 90-96, 2019 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30353791

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To assess missed opportunities for reducing fatal opioid overdoses, characteristics of decedents by opioid overdose with and without problematic opioid use who received health care services within one year of death were examined. METHODS: Of 157 decedents in the Worcester, Massachusetts, area between 2008 and 2012, 112 had contact with the health care system. Electronic medical records were reviewed for clinical characteristics, health service use, universal precautions, and substance use disorder management. Problematic opioid use was defined as individuals having documented opioid use disorders or aberrant drug-related behavior. Data were analyzed with chi-square tests with adjusted residual for categorical variables and t tests for continuous variables. RESULTS: Decedents were predominantly Caucasian males with a mean±SD age of 41.0±11.7. Problematic opioid use by definition meant users (N=53) had opioid use disorder as a principal diagnosis and were likely to have a comorbid substance use disorder. Decedents with nonproblematic opioid use had diagnoses of chronic pain and mental illness. They were more likely to have been seen last in surgical and subspecialty settings (29% versus 11%). The proportion with an opioid prescription was higher among those with problematic use (72% versus 37%) who also had a higher total daily morphine equivalent, compared with those with nonproblematic use (165.4±282.7 versus 55.6±117.7 mg per day). CONCLUSIONS: Persons with problematic opioid use are a recognizable group with a high risk of death by opioid overdose whose therapeutic management needs improvement to reduce fatal outcomes. Different strategies must be developed for identifying and treating nonproblematic opioid use to reduce risk of death.


Asunto(s)
Dolor Crónico/tratamiento farmacológico , Sobredosis de Droga/mortalidad , Prescripciones de Medicamentos/estadística & datos numéricos , Trastornos Mentales/mortalidad , Trastornos Relacionados con Opioides/mortalidad , Uso Excesivo de Medicamentos Recetados/estadística & datos numéricos , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/mortalidad , Población Blanca/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto , Dolor Crónico/epidemiología , Comorbilidad , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Massachusetts/epidemiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Neoplasias/mortalidad , Estudios Retrospectivos
5.
Am J Addict ; 26(8): 838-844, 2017 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29143399

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Opioid use disorder among young adults is rising sharply with an increase in morbidity and mortality. This study examined differences in treatment response to a fixed dose of buprenorphine-naloxone between heroin (HU) and prescriptions opioids (POU) users. METHODS: Eighty opioid dependent young adults (M = 22 years) were treated with buprenorphine-naloxone 16-4 mg/day for 8 weeks. Differences between HU (N = 17) and POU (N = 63) on changes in weekly opioid use, opioid craving, withdrawal, and depression symptoms were analyzed with mixed-effects regression models. RESULTS: The HU had an overall mean proportion of weekly opioid use of .32 (SD = .14) compared to POU's weekly mean of .24 (SD = .15) showing a significant main effect (Z = 2.21, p = .02). Depressive symptoms (CES-D scores) were elevated at baseline for both groups (HU: M = 23.1, SD = 11.9; PO: M = 22.2, SD = 9.4), but only POU improved significantly to a score of 9.88 (SD = 7.4) compared to HU's score of 18.58 (SD = 10.3) at week 8 (Z = 2.24, p = .02). There were no significant differences in treatment retention, craving, or withdrawal symptoms. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS: Treatment response to 16-4 mg/day of buprenorphine-naloxone was significantly diminished for heroin users relative to opioid prescription users in weekly opioid use. Heroin users also had persistent depressive symptoms suggesting the need for close monitoring. SCIENTIFIC SIGNIFICANCE: These data suggest that young heroin users might require higher doses of buprenorphine. (Am J Addict 2017;26:838-844).


Asunto(s)
Analgésicos Opioides/uso terapéutico , Combinación Buprenorfina y Naloxona/uso terapéutico , Dependencia de Heroína/rehabilitación , Tratamiento de Sustitución de Opiáceos/métodos , Trastornos Relacionados con Opioides/rehabilitación , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/rehabilitación , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual , Terapia Combinada , Ansia/efectos de los fármacos , Quimioterapia Combinada , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memantina/uso terapéutico , Psicoterapia de Grupo , Síndrome de Abstinencia a Sustancias/etiología , Adulto Joven
6.
J Dual Diagn ; 13(4): 298-304, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29120266

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Cue-elicited craving is a clinically important aspect of cocaine addiction directly linked to cognitive control breakdowns and relapse to cocaine-taking behavior. However, whether craving drives breakdowns in cognitive control toward cocaine cues in veterans, who experience significantly more co-occurring mood disorders, is unknown. The present study tests whether veterans have breakdowns in cognitive control because of cue-elicited craving or current anxiety or depression symptoms. METHODS: Twenty-four veterans with cocaine use disorder were cue-exposed, then tested on an antisaccade task in which participants were asked to control their eye movements toward cocaine or neutral cues by looking away from the cue. The relationship among cognitive control breakdowns (as measured by eye errors), cue-induced craving (changes in self-reported craving following cocaine cue exposure), and mood measures (depression and anxiety) was investigated. RESULTS: Veterans made significantly more errors toward cocaine cues than neutral cues. Depression and anxiety scores, but not cue-elicited craving, were significantly associated with increased subsequent errors toward cocaine cues for veterans. CONCLUSIONS: Increased depression and anxiety are specifically related to more cognitive control breakdowns toward cocaine cues in veterans. Depression and anxiety must be considered further in the etiology and treatment of cocaine use disorder in veterans. Furthermore, treating depression and anxiety as well, rather than solely alleviating craving levels, may prove a more effective combined treatment option in veterans with cocaine use disorder.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/complicaciones , Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Trastornos Relacionados con Cocaína/complicaciones , Trastornos Relacionados con Cocaína/psicología , Trastorno Depresivo/complicaciones , Trastorno Depresivo/psicología , Adulto , Afecto , Cocaína/administración & dosificación , Cognición , Ansia , Señales (Psicología) , Diagnóstico Dual (Psiquiatría) , Inhibidores de Captación de Dopamina/administración & dosificación , Función Ejecutiva , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Movimientos Oculares , Humanos , Entrevista Psicológica , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Actividad Motora , Pruebas Psicológicas , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Veteranos/psicología
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 234(7): 1967-1976, 2016 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26928432

RESUMEN

Attention plays a critical role in action selection. However, the role of attention in eye movements is complicated as these movements can be either voluntary or involuntary, with, in some circumstances (antisaccades), these two actions competing with each other for execution. But attending to the location of an impending eye movement is only one facet of attention that may play a role in eye movement selection. In two experiments, we investigated the effect of arousal on voluntary eye movements (antisaccades) and involuntary eye movements (prosaccadic errors) in an antisaccade task. Arousal, as caused by brief loud sounds and indexed by changes in pupil diameter, had a facilitation effect on involuntary eye movements. Involuntary eye movements were both significantly more likely to be executed and significantly faster under arousal conditions (Experiments 1 and 2), and the influence of arousal had a specific time course (Experiment 2). Arousal, one form of attention, can produce significant costs for human movement selection as potent but unplanned actions are benefited more than planned ones.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Volición/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Adulto Joven
8.
Addict Behav ; 52: 98-102, 2016 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26406974

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Many studies suggest that dependent smokers have a preference or attentional bias toward smoking cues. The purpose of this study was to test the ability of infrequent non-dependent light smokers to control their eye movements by look away from smoking cues. Poor control in the lightest of smokers would suggest nicotine cue-elicited behavior occurring even prior to nicotine dependency as measured by daily smoking. METHODS: 17 infrequent non-dependent light smokers and 17 lifetime non-smokers performed an antisaccade task (look away from suddenly appearing cue) on smoking, alcohol, neutral, and dot cues. RESULTS: The light smokers, who were confirmed light smokers and non-dependent (MFaegerström Dependency Score=0.35), were significantly worse at controlling their eye movements to smoking cues relative to both neutral cues (p<.04) and alcohol cues (p<.02). Light smokers made significantly more errors to smoking cues than non-smokers (p<.004). CONCLUSIONS: These data suggest that prior to developing clinical symptoms of severe dependence or progressing to heavier smoking (e.g., daily smoking), the lightest of smokers are showing a specific deficit in control of nicotine cue-elicited behavior.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Movimientos Oculares , Fumar/psicología , Adulto , Atención , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
9.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 156: 243-253, 2015 Nov 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26454835

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Opioid use disorders are considered a serious public health problem among young adults. Current treatment is limited to long-term opioid substitution therapy, with high relapse rates after discontinuation. This study evaluated the co-administration of memantine to brief buprenorphine pharmacotherapy as a treatment alternative. METHODS: 13-week double-blind placebo-controlled trial evaluating 80 young adult opioid dependent participants treated with buprenorphine/naloxone 16-4mg/day and randomized to memantine (15mg or 30mg) or placebo. Primary outcomes were a change in the weekly mean proportion of opioid use, and cumulative abstinence rates after rapid buprenorphine discontinuation on week 9. RESULTS: Treatment retention was not significantly different between groups. The memantine 30mg group was significantly less likely to relapse and to use opioids after buprenorphine discontinuation. Among participants abstinent on week 8, those in the memantine 30mg group (81.9%) were significantly less likely to relapse after buprenorphine was discontinued compared to the placebo group (30%) (p<0.025). Also, the memantine 30mg group had significantly reduced opioid use (mean=0, SEM±0.00) compared to the placebo group (mean=0.33, SEM±0.35; p<0.004) during the last 2 weeks of study participation. CONCLUSIONS: Memantine 30mg significantly improved short-term treatment with buprenorphine/naloxone for opioid dependent young adults by reducing relapse and opioid use after buprenorphine discontinuation. Combined short-term treatment with buprenorphine/naloxone may be an effective alternative treatment to long-term methadone or buprenorphine maintenance in young adults.


Asunto(s)
Combinación Buprenorfina y Naloxona/uso terapéutico , Memantina/uso terapéutico , Naloxona/uso terapéutico , Antagonistas de Narcóticos/uso terapéutico , Tratamiento de Sustitución de Opiáceos/métodos , Trastornos Relacionados con Opioides/rehabilitación , Adolescente , Adulto , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Método Doble Ciego , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Recurrencia , Adulto Joven
10.
Addict Behav ; 47: 86-90, 2015 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25900705

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Cue-induced craving is a clinically important aspect of cocaine addiction influencing ongoing use and sobriety. However, little is known about the relationship between cue-induced craving and cognitive control toward cocaine cues. While studies suggest that cocaine users have an attentional bias toward cocaine cues, the present study extends this research by testing if cocaine use disorder patients (CDPs) can control their eye movements toward cocaine cues and whether their response varied by cue-induced craving intensity. METHODS: Thirty CDPs underwent a cue exposure procedure to dichotomize them into high and low craving groups followed by a modified antisaccade task in which subjects were asked to control their eye movements toward either a cocaine or neutral drug cue by looking away from the suddenly presented cue. The relationship between breakdowns in cognitive control (as measured by eye errors) and cue-induced craving (changes in self-reported craving following cocaine cue exposure) was investigated. RESULTS: CDPs overall made significantly more errors toward cocaine cues compared to neutral cues, with higher cravers making significantly more errors than lower cravers even though they did not differ significantly in addiction severity, impulsivity, anxiety, or depression levels. Cue-induced craving was the only specific and significant predictor of subsequent errors toward cocaine cues. CONCLUSION: Cue-induced craving directly and specifically relates to breakdowns of cognitive control toward cocaine cues in CDPs, with higher cravers being more susceptible. Hence, it may be useful identifying high cravers and target treatment toward curbing craving to decrease the likelihood of a subsequent breakdown in control.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Relacionados con Cocaína/complicaciones , Trastornos Relacionados con Cocaína/psicología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/complicaciones , Trastornos del Conocimiento/psicología , Ansia , Señales (Psicología) , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
11.
Clin Psychol Rev ; 30(1): 12-24, 2010 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19748166

RESUMEN

Despite the declining overall rate of cigarette smoking in the general population in the United States, the prevalence of smoking is estimated to be as high as 80% among treatment-seeking alcoholics. The serious adverse health effects of tobacco and heavy alcohol use are synergistic and recent evidence suggests that smoking slows the process of cognitive recovery following alcohol abstinence. In addition, substantial evidence shows that treatment for tobacco dependence does not jeopardize alcohol abstinence. In this paper, we focus on the impact and treatment implications of tobacco dependence among treatment-seeking alcoholics through a review of five areas of research. We begin with brief reviews of two areas of research: studies investigating the genetic and neurobiological vulnerability of comorbid tobacco and alcohol dependence and studies investigating the consequences of comorbid dependence on neurobiological and cognitive functioning. We then review literature on the effects of smoking cessation on drinking urges and alcohol use and the effectiveness of smoking cessation interventions with alcoholic smokers. Finally, we offer recommendations for research with an emphasis on clinical research for enhancing smoking cessation outcomes in this population.


Asunto(s)
Alcoholismo/terapia , Cese del Hábito de Fumar/métodos , Tabaquismo/terapia , Alcoholismo/complicaciones , Terapia Conductista , Humanos , Centros de Tratamiento de Abuso de Sustancias , Tabaquismo/complicaciones
12.
ScientificWorldJournal ; 7: 626-40, 2007 May 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17619748

RESUMEN

Animal models of reward processing have revealed an extensive network of brain areas that process different aspects of reward, from expectation and prediction to calculation of relative value. These results have been confirmed and extended in human neuroimaging to encompass secondary rewards more unique to humans, such as money. The majority of the extant literature covers the brain areas associated with rewards whilst neglecting analysis of the actual behaviours that these rewards generate. This review strives to redress this imbalance by illustrating the importance of looking at the behavioural outcome of rewards and the context in which they are produced. Following a brief review of the literature of reward-related activity in the brain, we examine the effect of reward context on actions. These studies reveal how the presence of reward vs. reward and punishment, or being conscious vs. unconscious of reward-related actions, differentially influence behaviour. The latter finding is of particular importance given the extent to which animal models are used in understanding the reward systems of the human mind. It is clear that further studies are needed to learn about the human reaction to reward in its entirety, including any distinctions between conscious and unconscious behaviours. We propose that studies of reward entail a measure of the animal's (human or nonhuman) knowledge of the reward and knowledge of its own behavioural outcome to achieve that reward.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Recompensa , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Humanos
13.
Exp Brain Res ; 174(4): 786-92, 2006 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16977447

RESUMEN

Very little is known about how human movements are influenced by abstract rewards and punishments relevant for human behaviour. The purpose of this study was to expand our knowledge of the behavioural effects of monetary reward and punishment. We introduced a high and low reward and punishment scheme into an antisaccade task where trials were either rewarded for a correct response (+1 or +25p) or punished for an incorrect response (-1 or -25p). The monetary value of the trial was indicated by the go signal, so subjects had to both program the location of the movement and determine the valence in the short interval before the eye movement was executed. We analysed both correct antisaccade responses and prosaccade errors. Importantly, the errors in this task can be either conscious (recognised) or unconscious (unrecognised). Saccades in both high-reward and high-punishment trials were slowed compared to saccades in low-reward and low-punishment trials, respectively. Therefore, unlike moderate rewards only (Blaukopf and DiGirolamo in Exp Brain Res 167:654-659, 2005), combining rewards and punishments and increasing motivation levels leads to a delay in movement execution during high valence trials where all actions are slowed, even errors. However, unconscious errors were differentially affected as they were speeded when punishment was high. We conclude that reward and punishment similarly influence the programming of conscious movements, but the strong saliency for punishment affords unconscious errors immunity from this delay.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Castigo , Recompensa , Inconsciente en Psicología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
14.
Exp Brain Res ; 167(4): 654-9, 2005 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16284757

RESUMEN

In a gap antisaccade task that exogenously cues the side that subjects should antisaccade to, subjects find it hard to look away from the suddenly appearing go signal. Surprisingly, subjects are unaware of the majority of the prosaccade errors they make, and these errors remain unrecognised even when corrected by a second saccade requiring twice the amplitude [Fischer B, Weber H (1992) in Exp Brain Res 89:415-424]. This paper presents an extended antisaccade task that investigates what information, if any, subjects extract from redundant cues and go signals. In Exp. 1, multiple saccade locations were introduced and the go signal specified the goal location. A redundant cue appeared, prior to the go signal, in the antisaccade goal location (valid) or in the alternative location on the same side (invalid). In Exp. 2, motivational value was assigned to the go signal. The use of multiple locations showed that subjects automatically extract irrelevant positional information from the cue, which affects the programming of subsequent correct and error saccades. When the cued location was also the goal location, antisaccade reaction times were significantly reduced. The results from Exp. 2 showed that subjects also extract information from the go signal. Errors made to a go signal associated with a higher monetary value were initiated significantly faster than those to a lower monetary value. This study has shown that the visual stimuli used in this antisaccade task do more than initiate orienting sets: Their properties can influence the programming of both accurate actions and errors.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
15.
J Vis ; 3(11): 751-60, 2003 Dec 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14765958

RESUMEN

Research has produced conflicting evidence as to whether saccade programming is or is not biased by perceptual illusions. However, previous studies have generally not distinguished between effects of illusory percepts on reflexive saccades, programmed automatically in response to an external visual signal, and voluntary saccades, programmed purposively to a location where no signal has occurred. Here we find that voluntary and reflexive saccades are differentially susceptible to the Müller-Lyer illusion; reflexive movements are reliably but modestly affected by the illusion, whereas voluntary movements show an effect similar to that of perceptual judgments. Results suggest that voluntary saccade programming occurs within a non-retinotopic spatial representation similar to that of visual consciousness, whereas reflexive saccade programming occurs within a representation integrating retinotopic and higher level spatial frames. The effects of the illusion on reflexive saccades are not subject to endogenous control, nor are they modulated by the strength of an exogenous target signal.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Humanos , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...